Report
Money can’t buy love: Asymmetric beliefs about gift price and feelings of appreciation

https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2008.11.003Get rights and content

Abstract

Across three studies, we identify an asymmetry between gift-givers’ and gift-recipients’ beliefs about the link between gift price and feelings of appreciation. Gift-givers expected a positive correlation between how much they spent on a gift and the extent to which gift-recipients would appreciate the gift because gift-givers assume that more expensive gifts convey a higher level of thoughtfulness. Gift-recipients, in contrast, reported no such association between gift price and their actual feelings of appreciation. This effect occurred regardless of whether the individual’s role and the magnitude of the gift were manipulated or measured in the field. Taken together, these findings cast doubt on whether gift-givers can draw on their personal experience as gift-recipients in order to identify meaningful gifts for others.

Introduction

Every day, millions of shoppers hit the stores in full force—both online and on foot—searching frantically for the perfect gift. Last year, Americans spent over $30 billion at retail stores in the month of December alone. Aside from purchasing holiday gifts, most people regularly buy presents for other occasions throughout the year, including weddings, birthdays, anniversaries, graduations, and baby showers. This frequent experience of gift-giving can engender ambivalent feelings in gift-givers. Many relish the opportunity to buy presents because gift-giving offers a powerful means to build stronger bonds with one’s closest peers. At the same time, many dread the thought of buying gifts; they worry that their purchases will disappoint rather than delight the intended recipients.

Anthropologists describe gift-giving as a positive social process, serving various political, religious, and psychological functions (e.g., Boas, 1895, Mauss, 1925). Economists, however, offer a less favorable view. According to Waldfogel (1993), gift-giving represents an objective waste of resources. People buy gifts that recipients would not choose to buy on their own, or at least not spend as much money to purchase (a phenomenon referred to as “the deadweight loss of Christmas”). To wit, givers are likely to spend $100 to purchase a gift that receivers would spend only $80 to buy themselves. This “deadweight loss” suggests that gift-givers are not very good at predicting what gifts others will appreciate. That in itself is not surprising to social psychologists. Research has found that people often struggle to take account of others’ perspectives—their insights are subject to egocentrism, social projection, and multiple attribution errors.

What is surprising is that gift-givers have considerable experience acting as both gift-givers and gift-recipients, but nevertheless tend to overspend each time they set out to purchase a meaningful gift. In the present research, we propose a unique psychological explanation for this overspending problem—that gift-givers equate how much they spend with how much recipients will appreciate the gift (i.e., the more expensive the gift, the stronger a gift-recipient’s feelings of appreciation). Although a link between gift price and feelings of appreciation might seem intuitive to gift-givers, such an assumption may be unfounded. Indeed, we propose that gift-recipients will be less inclined to base their feelings of appreciation on the magnitude of a gift than givers assume.

Why do gift-givers assume that gift price is closely linked to gift-recipients’ feelings of appreciation? Perhaps givers believe that bigger (i.e., more expensive) gifts convey stronger signals of thoughtfulness and consideration. According to Camerer (1988) and others (e.g., Malinowski, 1922, Mauss, 1925), gift-giving represents a symbolic ritual, whereby gift-givers attempt to signal their positive attitudes toward the intended recipient and their willingness to invest resources in a future relationship. In this sense, gift-givers may be motivated to spend more money on a gift in order to send a “stronger signal” to their intended recipient. As for gift-recipients, they may not construe smaller and larger gifts as representing smaller and larger signals of thoughtfulness and consideration (see Ames, Flynn, and Weber (2004)).

The notion of gift-givers and gift-recipients being unable to account for the other party’s perspective seems puzzling because people slip in and out of these roles every day, and, in some cases, multiple times in the course of the same day. Yet, despite the extensive experience that people have as both givers and receivers, they often struggle to transfer information gained from one role (e.g., as a giver) and apply it in another, complementary role (e.g., as a receiver). In theoretical terms, people fail to utilize information about their own preferences and experiences in order to produce more efficient outcomes in their exchange relations. In practical terms, people spend hundreds of dollars each year on gifts, but somehow never learn to calibrate their gift expenditures according to personal insight. Ironically, it seems the giver/receiver role, and the unique psychological experience that accompanies it, may serve as a natural roadblock to developing healthy patterns of social exchange.

We make three central predictions. First, we propose that gift-givers will associate a gift-recipient’s feelings of appreciation with the price of the gift—the more the giver spends, the stronger the anticipated feelings of appreciation. Second, the same association will not hold for gift-recipients. That is, the link between gift price and feelings of appreciation will be stronger for gift-givers than for gift-recipients. Third, for givers, thoughtfulness will mediate the link between gift price and anticipated feelings of appreciation.

We tested these ideas in three studies: two that investigated people’s reactions to actual gift purchases and one that experimentally manipulated a hypothetical gift-giving scenario.

Section snippets

Study 1: Engagement rings

This study examined whether gift-givers are more inclined than gift-recipients to associate gift price with recipients’ feelings of appreciation. To test this asymmetric prediction, we collected data from recently engaged individuals who had either purchased or received an engagement ring.

Study 2: Birthday gifts

In Study 2, we asked people to describe a gift they had either given or received for another individual’s (or their own) birthday. Once again, we predicted that givers would connect gift price to their estimates of appreciation, but that recipients would show no such relationship. Further, we expected that this finding would remain robust when controlling for social desirability bias.

Study 3: Gift-giving Vignette

According to the results of Studies 1 and 2, when givers buy more expensive gifts, they believe these items will be more highly appreciated, but gift-recipients do not relate gift price to their actual feelings of appreciation. In Study 3, we examine this idea more rigorously, with participants randomly assigned to different roles in the hypothetical exchange of either a small or large gift.

General discussion

We found that gift-givers consistently overestimate the extent to which feelings of appreciation are driven by gift price. Whereas gift-givers assumed that gift-recipients would be more appreciative of larger gifts than smaller ones, the recipients in our studies did not agree. Instead, their reactions to the larger gifts were not significantly stronger than their reactions to the smaller (i.e., less expensive) gifts. In contrast to previous research on gift exchange that has typically asked

Broader implications and future directions

Popular theories of exchange, such as social exchange theory (e.g., Thibaut & Kelley, 1959) and equity theory (e.g., Adams, 1965), presume that people think about exchange as cost-benefit accounting. According to this instrumental view, one’s attitudes about gift-giving should hinge on the balance of material benefits. Our results, and other recent findings (e.g., Flynn & Brockner, 2003), challenge this assumption. In the present research, we found that gift-givers’ weighing of instrumental

Possible interventions

What might gift-givers do to purchase better gifts, avoiding the “deadweight loss” that economists lament? One suggestion is straightforward: purchase smaller gifts. Families, coworkers, and friendship networks could be better served by establishing maximum spending limits in order to curtail excessive and wasteful gift-giving. With spending constraints in place, gift-givers may work harder to identify meaningful, rather than magnificent, presents. Many generous gift-givers hope to elicit

Conclusion

Gift-givers assume that gift-recipients’ feelings of appreciation are closely tied to the amount of money spent on a gift, but this does not appear to be the case. Instead, the signaling value of a more expensive gift, which givers hope will elicit appreciation, may be lost on most recipients. At first blush, it seems surprising that being in a giver role or a receiver role can lead to such dramatic differences in people’s expectations and actual feelings of appreciation, given that people slip

References (25)

  • C. Heath

    On the social psychology of agency relationships: Lay theories of motivation overemphasize extrinsic rewards

    Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes

    (1999)
  • S. Adams

    Inequity in social exchange

  • L.S. Aiken et al.

    Multiple regression: Testing and interpreting interactions

    (1991)
  • D. Ames et al.

    It’s the thought that counts: On perceiving how favor-givers decide to help

    Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin

    (2004)
  • R.M. Baron et al.

    The moderator–mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1986)
  • F. Boas

    Indian legends of the Northwest Coast

    (1895)
  • C. Camerer

    Gifts as economic signals and social symbols

    American Journal of Sociology

    (1988)
  • T. Caplow

    Christmas gifts and kin networks

    American Sociological Review

    (1982)
  • N. Cotterell et al.

    Inhibiting effects of reciprocation wariness on interpersonal relationships

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (1992)
  • N. Epley et al.

    Perspective taking as egocentric anchoring and adjustment

    Journal of Personality and Social Psychology

    (2004)
  • R.J. Fisher

    Social desirability bias and the validity of indirect questioning

    Journal of Consumer Research

    (1993)
  • R.J. Fisher et al.

    Social desirability bias of the validity of self-reported values

    Psychology and Marketing

    (2000)
  • Cited by (90)

    • When brokers don't broker: Mitigating referral aversion in third-party help exchange

      2024, Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes
    • The Misprediction of Helpers in Comforting Situations and Its Mechanism

      2024, Psychology Research and Behavior Management
    View all citing articles on Scopus
    View full text